Ask an Israeli about the conflict with Palestinians and you'll probably hear a tale of woe and victimhood. The strange thing is, if you ask a Palestinian the same question, you'll most likely hear the same story. Since Israel became a nation in 1948, both Israelis and Palestinians have felt victimized.
From the 1949 Armistice Agreements to the most recent Oslo peace negotiations (1993), any formal attempt for a lasting and comprehensive peace settlement between Israelis and Palestinians has spectacularly failed. Although economic, historical, and cultural factors are important starting points for understanding this conflict, the rationale for perpetuating it today turns on how each side answers one simple line of questioning: Who is the greatest victim? Who has suffered more?
Victimhood experiences have dramatic consequences for the relations between communities (e.g., Bar-Tal, Chernyak-Hai, Schori, & Gundar, 2009), as demonstrated by the Arab-Israel conflict or by other long-term conflicts around the world (for example, former Yugoslavia and North-Ireland). Further, Masi Noor and colleagues (see Noor, Schnabel, Samer, & Nadler, in press, for a review) suggests that, especially in contexts where material and social resources are scarce, group members actively attempt to affirm that one’s own group has been victimized more than the other. This group tendency, called competitive victimhood, plays a key role in decreasing the prospect for future peaceful coexistence between conflicting groups.
To promote reconciliation and end conflict, it is vitally important to overcome competitive victimhood. Here's why.
The hidden obstacle to reconciliation
How can members of different groups live together after decades of mutual violence, humiliation and abuse? Is it possible for them to coexist peacefully? Addressing these questions is perhaps one of the most challenging but urgent tasks for social scientists in general, and social psychologists in particular. Over the last few decades, the world has indeed been marked by deep-rooted conflicts, not just across national borders, but also between ethnic and religious groups within the same territory. The Arab-Israel conflict, Apartheid in South Africa, genocide in Rwanda, violence between Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland, and civil wars in the former Yugoslavia are just a few examples.
What is clear is that formal agreements between political leaders or strategic divisions of valuable resources, such as money or land, are not enough. For most of these conflicts, diplomatic and strategic negotiations have proved to be a feeble panacea. After the cessation of hostilities, conflicting groups often return to common spaces, but feelings of distrust, lack of empathy and increasing motivations for revenge remain potent even after the formal resolution of the conflict, and may trigger an endless cycle of violence (Nadler, 2002).
Only a radical change in each group's perception of the others can break the cycle of violence and guarantee an enduring peace. This notion is the core of reconciliation (Staub, 2006). More specifically, reconciliation implies the mutual acceptance by groups of each other (e.g., Staub & Bar-Tal, 2003). This is a long and hard process that should involve all the segments of the conflicting groups. And it can be achieved only by addressing each of the parties’ conflict-related feelings, thoughts, and needs (Nadler, Malloy, & Fisher, 2008).
The intrinsic need to compete over victimhood is perhaps one of the greatest inhibitors of reconciliation processes, and removing it can crucially contribute to an enduring peace. Fortunately, social psychologists have suggested strategies that could dismantle the phenomenon. But first, let's take a closer look at it.
Bar-Tal., D. (2007). Living with the conflict: Socio-psychological analysis of the Israeli-Jewish society. Jerusalem: Carmel (in Hebrew).
Bar-Tal, D., Chernyak-Hai, L., Schori, N., & Gundar, A. (2009). A sense of self-perceived victimhood in intractable conflicts.International Review of the Red Cross, 91, 229-258.
Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 554–594). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bruneau, E. G., & Saxe, R. (2012). The power of being heard: The benefits of ‘perspective-giving’ in the context of
intergroup conflict . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 855-866.
Buckley-Zistel, S. (2006). Remembering to forget: Chosen amnesia as a strategy for local coexistence in post-genocide Rwanda. The Journal of The International African Institute, 76, 131-150.
Cairns, E., Mallet, J., Lewis, C., & Wilson, R. (2003). Who are the victims? Self-assessed victimhood and the Northern-Irish conflict. NIO Research and Statistical Series, Report No.7. Belfast: NISRA.
Caplan, N. (1999). Victimhood and identity: Psychological obstacles to Israeli reconciliation with the Palestinians’. In K. Abdel-Malek & D. C. Jacobson (Eds.), Israeli and Palestinian identities in history and literature (pp.63-86). New York: St Martin’s Press.
Crisp, R. J., Stathi, S., Turner, R. N., & Husnu, S. (2008). Imagined intergroup contact: Theory,
paradigm, and practice.Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1-18.
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Saguy, T. (2009). Commonality and the complexity of “we”: Social attitudes and social change. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 3-20.
Gaunt, R. (2009). Superordinate
categorization as a moderator of mutual infra-humanization. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12, 731-746.
Nadler, A., Malloy, T. E., & Fisher, J. D. (Eds.). (2008). Social psychology of intergroup reconciliation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Nadler, A., & Saguy, T. (2003). Reconciliation between nations: Overcoming emotional deterrents to ending conflicts between groups. In H. Langholtz & C. E. Stout (Eds.), The psychology of diplomacy (pp. 29-46). Westport, CT: Praege.
Noor, M., Brown, J. R., Gonzalez, R., Manzi, J., & Lewis, C. A. (2008). On positive psychological outcomes: What helps groups with a history of conflict to forgive and reconcile with each other? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,819-832.
Noor, M., Brown, J. R., & Prentice, G. (2008). Precursors and mediators of intergroup reconciliation in Northern Ireland: A new model. Journal of British Social Psychology, 47, 481-495.
Noor, M., Shnabel, N., Halabi, S., & Nadler, A. (in press). When suffering begets suffering: The psychology of competitive victimhood between adversarial groups in violent conflicts. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
Paolini, S., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., & Voci, A. (2004). Effects of direct and indirect cross-group friendships on judgments of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland: The mediating role of an anxiety-reduction mechanism. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 770-786.
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. (2006). A meta-
analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751-783.
Simon, B. & Klandermans, B. (2001). Politicized collective identity: A social psychological analysis. American Psychologist, 56, 319-31.
Smyth, M. (2001). Putting the past in its place: Issues of victimhood and reconciliation in Northern Ireland's peace process. In N. Biggar (Ed.), Burying the past: Making peace and doing justice after civil conflict (pp. 125-155). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
Staub, E. (2006). Reconciliation after genocide, mass killing, or intractable conflict: Understanding the roots of violence, psychological recovery, and steps toward a general theory. Political Psychology, 867-894.
Staub, E., & Bar-Tal, D. (2003). Genocide, mass killing and intractable conflict: Roots, evolution, prevention and reconciliation. In D. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jarvis (Eds.), Handbook of political psychology (pp. 710–755). NewYork: Oxford University Press.
Volkan, V. (1997). Blood lines: From ethnic pride to ethnic terrorism. Boulder, Co: Westview Press.
Volkan, V. (1997). Blood lines: From ethnic pride to ethnic terrorism. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Wohl, M. J. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2005). Forgiveness and collective guilt assignment to historical perpetrator groups depends on level of social category inclusiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 288-303.
Wright, S. C., Aron, A., McLaughlin-Volpe, T., & Ropp, S. A. (1997). The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and
prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 73-90.